Doing research among family
and friends.
Problems and advantages
Pihla Vuorinen
Within the family (1)
many stories are told as a part of common reminiscing about the
sayings and doings of family members in different times. I decided
to do research on this subject, which is familiar to everyone,
partly through my own experiences and in my own family. People
taking part in my research on family stories (see Vuorinen 2000)
have mostly been either members of my own family or my friends
with their families. I have collected my material via a questionnaire
and an individual or a group interview, using also my own memories,
stories and experiences as research material. (2) In this
article I will discuss the benefits and specific problems related
to the closeness of both the material and the people it is collected
from.
The growing interest towards
one's own family is discernible also in Finland. Making genealogies
and doing research on the history of one's own family has become
more and more popular. However, doing research among your own
close ones was not a common practice until last two decades.
Folklorists and other researchers have rarely approached family-related
questions through their own experiences and families. Instead,
fieldwork is usually done amongst people one does not know. In
America conducting fieldwork within one's own family has longer
traditions, and also the related problems have been discussed
(see e.g. Miller 1997; Sherman 1986; Scheiberg 1990; Wilson 1991;
also Holbek 1990). (3)
According to Susan Scheiberg,
folklore students are encouraged to do research among their family
and friends. The researcher benefits from knowing her (4)
co-participants well and being able to rely on shared experiences,
which is of assistance in the process of fieldwork. They share
strong mutual trust, liking and co-operation (Scheiberg 1990:
208). These often mentioned benefits of doing fieldwork at home
are related to cultural competence. There are things that are
difficult - if not impossible - to learn for an outsider and
for example inner traditions of families can be of this nature.
Subjectivity and emotional
understanding
Researchers' own experiences
have not traditionally been particularly valued as a source of
knowledge. Objectivity and subjectivity have been seen as mutually
exclusive categories, and namely objectivity has been the basis
for scientific research. This premise is based on the conception
that the self is independent from the others, and objectivity
of research is achieved by distancing the self from others. The
ideology of objectivity is found for example as dominance and
emotional distance in fieldwork relationships and as alienation
of the self of the researcher from the other of the subjects
(Camitta 1990: 22-23).
The ideal has been to distance
the personality of the researcher while writing a study. Personal
has meant the same as unscientific (see e.g. Suojanen 1995: 158).
Before the change of research paradigm (in connection with transition
to post-modern period), including information about the researcher
herself in the study has not only been unscientific, but also
inappropriate, excessively personal and trivial. Subjective elements
originating from the researcher should be hidden to the text
with different kinds of methods (Vasenkari 1996: 86-87; cf. also
Clifford & Marcus 1986). If fieldwork was conducted in one's
own community, even in one's own family, this fact was not mentioned
in the report.
[Stoeltje] had begun to
carry out folklore research in my "own" culture, collecting
stories from my father. [---] However, I did not mention
my fieldwork or my life history as the basis for the article
[1975]. At that point of time, it was acceptable to conduct
fieldwork in one's own community and even in one's own family,
but it was not yet conventional to acknowledge these relationships
in print. (Stoeltje et al. 1999: 169)
It is more difficult to justify
the significance of your research, when you are doing it through
yourself and your close ones. Usually, referring to the informants
as friends seems to undermine objectivity and the value of research
in question. However, particularly the (close) friendship relations
are emotionally rewarding and bring along trust and reciprocity.
These kinds of relations also offer rich contexts for fertile
intercourse, learning and growth, and the understanding of the
other can be more than vicarious (see Camitta 1990: 25-26).
In my work, I have been striving
towards a sort of emotional understanding or 'empathic reading',
which Anni Vilkko (1995: 163) has described as follows:
For me empathic reading
is responsive, experiencing reading, "experiencing with
another" so that the interlocutor's world and the structures
that outline it would take shape, and understanding both likeness
and difference would be possible. It is not necessarily based
on identification and experiencing affinity. It is more like
an attitude, which gives room and possibility to see both affinity
and especially difference.
In my opinion, a prerequisite
for understanding in a broader sense is empathy - experiential
presence, living and feeling along. This kind of approach gives
a possibility to see similarities as well as differences, place
oneself in the position of informants, and see things from different
perspectives. This kind of attitude also gives a chance for growing
self-knowledge.
William Wilson has suggested
that studies should be based on personal material, because paying
particular attention to one's own family stories helps to understand
the feelings connected to the stories also on a wider scale.
"I shall discuss my data both passionately and at great
risk of exposure. And I shall do so because I do not believe
we can understand the emotional force narratives might exert
in the lives of others until we have dealt with that force as
honestly as possible in our own lives" (Wilson 1991: 130).
We can say that it is the emotions
that pass on family folklore; stories are adopted from those
persons one feels positively about and with whom one wants to
identify. Feelings related to the stories are important also
in shaping the relations between family members and building
identities. Usually the stories, which are used to create a picture
of growing up into oneself, include strong personal memories
and associations.
[---] These stories are
important to me, since they are so personal: I was 1 year 2 months
old when my mother left [to another town] to study. I
had to be separated from mother for up to 2 weeks, for which
I was far too little. Really thoughtless from my mother, but
what can you do. A couple of years ago in the attic of our summer
cottage I came across my baby development book, where mother
had written about one month before leaving [to study]. It
was something like this: We were in a singing event, and Ilona-baby
was also sitting on mother's knee and groped for the words of
the song holding a blue booklet in her hand. [---] "Hiljaa,
hiljaa joulunkellot kajahtaa ..." [Softly, softly, Christmas
bells are chiming]. Grandmother was taking care of me while
mother was [studying], and told afterwards many times,
how good girl I had been and in the evening I had asked grandmother
to sing this above-mentioned song. In the morning grandmother
had found me in my room bracing against the crib and singing:
"Hiijaa, hiijaa". In my mind I was probably on my mother's
arms. I like the story, though almost every time someone talks
about it I am moved to tears. When there are no symbols, music
makes the absent things concretely present. As a child it was
a coping strategy for me. [---] An analogous story was
told by aunt Anna, who had watched me trying to get into older
cousins' plays in every possible way, but I was always left out.
Finally I had given up and started to walk alone in the cornfield.
I was so small that only a wake could be seen where I walked
- and sang. (N34) (5)
Examining one's own family narratives the researcher gets to
know the privacy of these stories. By starting with the familiar
(subjective) material and one's own group, the researcher is
also less apt to objectify other people she will investigate
(Stoeltje et al. 1999: 160-161).
Combining two different
roles
Using one's own experiences
and, in this case, the stories of one's own family as a research
material has both advantages and disadvantages. The stories of
my own family as well as the people and events connected to them
are familiar and close to me, and in that sense easily approachable.
My relatives have taken my research project mostly positively
- maybe it would have been difficult for them not to answer to
my inquiries. People have been astonished chiefly because of
the interest I have shown towards the things usually perceived
as ordinary and even uninteresting. There has been a question
in the air if it is possible at all to make a thesis on this
kind of theme. Still, although the attitude towards my study
has been positive, it does not always mean that the time for
the interviews or writing down answers to my questionnaire was
easily found (cf. Sherman 1986: 55). Everyone has their own duties,
and especially situations, where all the family (or even most
of it) would be present, are rare. There is always a lot to do
at grandmother's place, as well as a lot to talk about when meeting
sisters after a long time. Making an interview cuts down the
infrequent time spent together.
Sometimes I have heard half-joking
comments from my family members, like: "Again those family
stories, haven't the stories of our family already been told?"
While interviewing my mother, my younger sisters quite quickly
got tired of listening or participating in the conversation,
and asked a couple of times if we were not already done with
the interview. Tape-recorded sessions of family stories did somehow
break the routines and the usual social intercourse of the family.
Sometimes I would have preferred just to enjoy spending time
with my family, and maybe leave the research work for some other
time (cf. Scheiberg 1990: 211: "Sometimes we want simply
to enjoy our family, not investigate it!"). When studying
my own family, my private and working life got intermingled,
and the borders between the roles of a researcher and a family
member became blurred (cf. Pink 2000).
The same kind of groping between
the two roles was experienced by Tuija Saarinen, who collected
material for her master's thesis (Saarinen 1993) from her home
village, inspired by the stories she had heard about Heikan Jussi
(a village shoemaker with a peculiar character) when she was
a kid. She has also encountered and discussed the difficulties
in negotiating and operating in terms of the roles of a researcher
and a family member or friend. Saarinen writes that the informants
received and greeted a researcher and a relative or friend at
the same time, and sometimes preferred to talk about subjects
other than the actual topic of the interview. Saarinen notes
that sometimes she felt guilty of organising the meeting just
to collect research material. Also the observation of the close
ones and writing a fieldwork diary was sometimes disconcerting
(Saarinen 1993: 23-25).
I experienced similar feelings
during the research process of my own. I sometimes asked myself
if I had a right (and to what extent) to make use of the goodwill
of my relatives and friends, and what the participation in the
research process could possibly give to them.
When doing research among the
intimates, the researcher has knowledge of her co-participants'
behaviour and can call on shared experiences to aid in the process
of fieldwork (Scheiberg 1990: 208). Also other researchers have
become aware of the difficulties arising from combining everyday
life with research. Especially while doing research among friends
and family, the researcher is forced to combine the two roles
and the expectations connected with them. The roles of a folklorist
and a family member are dissimilar, and a researcher-family member
has to shuttle between different (or even opposing) expectations
and hopes. Expectations have developed over time, and intimates
know the researcher first and foremost as a friend or a family
member: daughter, granddaughter, sister, etc. (see Scheiberg
1990: 209).
Negotiating and operating in
terms of two roles includes its own special problems, one of
these being the question of dual identity. The researcher is
at the same time an insider and an outsider, in a way a bit of
both, whatever one's identity might be. The researcher has to
be able to negotiate relationships, and accept the fact that
she will be defined as an outsider, when she begins research
in a familiar culture (Stoeltje et al. 1999: 177-178).
Kim Miller has remarked that
a folklorist doing research in her own family is expected as
a family member to be a 'performer' as well: "If she becomes
an observer or even shifts her degree of participation, other
family members will notice it and may display their disapproval"
(Miller 1997: 336). While interviewing family members, I have
also been bound to think over my own role in the interview. When
interviewing people whom I did not know before I am naturally
more an outsider, asking questions and observing. Especially
in group interviews conversation goes on between participants,
family members, and my role is rather noncommittal. When it comes
to my own family, the situation is different: I am myself part
of the group and the topics and stories discussed are well known
to me. The family members may find it strange to repeat the stories
they know I am already familiar with. Often my task has been
to encourage them to tell these stories again. Also there can
be things that would be easier to tell a stranger who does not
comment on them or take sides.
Due to the familiarity of the
stories, I have had a different role - that of an initiator -
while interviewing members of my own family. The questions I
ask are more detailed than while interviewing other people -
I know better what to ask. (6) However,
these situations have felt to some extent strange to me. Normally
I take part in the conversation and storytelling without analysing
the situation further. Now my aim was more to get other people
to speak and tell stories, and not to lead the course of conversation
too much. I was hoping to reach a kind of everyday conversational
situation. On the other hand, the search for a 'normal' conversation
was now disturbed - among other things - by my own special role
and the reservedness it brought along. As Miller writes, the
compromise of roles is often difficult to manage. Although trying
to make the interview a family conversation, she notes she was
only partly successful: "The interview ended up being an
interview with some of the attributes of a conversation"
(Miller 1997: 337).
The negative sides of family
stories
Studying one's own family,
the researcher also needs to (or cannot help to) consider what
she is willing to tell about herself publicly - if she is willing
to make all the personal, 'stupid' stories available to everyone
and how the presented data might affect others' views of her
(cf. Scheiberg 1990: 211). Besides the researcher's own, also
her family's privacy is at stake. Because the stories that are
told belong to the folklorist as much as to other family members,
it can also be easier for her to forget or ignore privacy issues
(Miller 1997: 333).
While selecting quotations
from the material, one more easily or even automatically censors
subjects delicate for oneself, but it is more difficult to estimate
the feelings and sentiments of other family members. However,
their reactions also have to be taken into consideration when
interpreting stories. As to the closeness of the material, interpretation
is usually easier, but the question is, how deep analyses one
can publicly make without offending the close ones, hoping to
maintain good relationships with them in future as well. Another
danger is that the researcher assumes she already knows how others
interpret the stories and how they feel about them. Actually,
the interpretations can vary over time, and different family
members may have divergent interpretations of the same story.
One's own family is a problematic research subject in many ways,
and I did not want to found my study on that context only.
One of the problems that relate
to conducting fieldwork among intimates is the possible negative
side of family folklore. Margaret Yocom has stated that because
many of the family folklorists in America worked with their own
families and were bound by family requests for privacy, little
discussion of the painful side of family life emerged until researchers
turned to studying other families (Yocom 1997: 280). In my opinion
it was more natural and easy to talk about the negative sides
of family life specifically with my own family. While interviewing
families of my friends the negative stories were usually only
alluded to, and it seemed to me that they did not want to discuss
them in detail when an outsider was present. Also the presence
of several family members in the interview might have hampered
pondering the difficult matters. If people have different opinions
about the course of events, they do not necessarily want to argue
about them 'publicly'. Interviewees could also be worried about
the possible misinterpretations of negative stories. As one of
the respondents writes:
Family stories are quite
rarely told to outsiders, mainly only to people "just about
to become part of the family" (who do not understand anything
about them); maybe the reason for this is that some of the stories
might sound quite negative, although people nearly always have
a benevolent attitude towards them. (M9)
In my opinion people in general
tell less about their negative memories, and stories of this
kind are told at more intimate, confidential person-to-person
moments: "I can't remember that negative things would have
been discussed that much in our family, rather they have been
revealed by mistake and indirectly" (N19). Exceptions to
this rule are stories, which are used to vilify someone in the
front of a larger public, to give a certain picture of a disliked
relative.
On the other hand, people do
reminisce how they overcame difficulties, survived, even though
these stories may include sad or negative elements: "Those
stories that are told (repeatedly), are usually about events
that at least afterwards seem to be positive, although I can
also remember so-called hopeless situations, which have somehow
been handled" (M9). Also unpleasant and sad things can sometimes
be laughed at afterwards. Humour is a kind of safeguard with
which even painful things can be discussed self-ironically.
In some written answers also
the painful sides of storytelling have been analysed very profoundly
and personally. These writings are more like diary reflections,
not meant for other relatives to read.
My family has always communicated
quite a lot with the closest relatives: with grandparents and
siblings of my parents. Especially relatives on my father's side
have been "part of our family". In adolescence this
annoyed me tremendously, a bit later, when I was about to move
away from home, relationships with the relatives from paternal
side were very exacerbated. I very rarely met relatives on my
mother's side, because I never really went anywhere with my parents.
As far as family stories are concerned, it seems peculiar to
me that I best remember the moments around tea-table from the
time when the relations between me and my father were not good,
and I did not spend so much time at home. But when I was there
and the above-mentioned people were present, those were the situations
when we told most stories. My father and my big sister (who has
always been "the father's girl") were like outsiders.
Well, sometimes my sister was with us. After the years of the
cold war my relations towards father and relatives from his side
of the family improved. And as long as they do not interfere
in my affairs, these fairly good relations remain or even get
better. Nowadays I even have surprisingly many things to talk
about with my father. But still family storytelling with him
does not work out. Maybe because of the grudge this reminiscence
might bring forward. There is no use of these stories, they are
not relevant when it comes to the meaning family stories in my
opinion have: nearness, cosiness, togetherness and building some
kind of positive self-image through memories and stories. (N17)
Although within one's own family
the discussing of negative things has been more natural, this
intimate side of storytelling has been more difficult to face
in the interview situations and more complicated to link with
research discourse. It is surprisingly difficult to publicly
write about things and people that are close to you. Studying
intimate material brings along fear of being uninteresting, too
personal and trivial. Much is said in the family circle that
is 'off the record', meant for the ears of the family members
only, but not for those of others, who would not understand it
(cf. Scheiberg 1990: 210).
[Family stories] emphasise
the difference in a person's behaviour in family circle and in
public: many things said and done in a family circle are not
meant for the ears of outsiders, and on the other hand they reinforce
bonding within the family. (N16)
There are many things that
the researcher is aware of because she is a family member. Also
I have lived surrounded by the stories and memories of my family
ever since I was a child and people passing me family traditions
have not thought of me as a researcher. Sometimes things have
been told in a very confidential way, and they have not been
meant to be analysed in public. The question is, to what extent
a researcher can use the information acquired this way as research
material. In principle she should not misuse the position of
a granddaughter, cousin, sister, friend, etc. (cf. Ruotsala 2001:
124).
As Miller has pointed out,
once words are published, they are available to everyone, and
they are irretrievable. In this way they carry more weight, and
they can do much harm (Miller 1997: 332). The meanings and emotions
connected to the stories cannot always be expressed verbally
at all, and written interpretation inevitably gives a one-sided
picture of multiform reality. What is more, family members do
not have any chance to comment on the stories and interpretations,
to come out with their own opinions after the study is published.
The written text does not change either when family stories,
traditions and relationships do. For example, negative stories
can be told in certain situations in order to anger or to hurt
someone and later, as the life situation changes, they can be
forgotten or reinterpreted.
I also encountered unhappy
stories and negative or sad feelings while conducting fieldwork.
I was bound to consider, for example, whether or not to bring
up certain topics (during the interview or in the research paper),
if I know they will make a family member cry. This kind of question
always calls for contextual consideration - how essential and
important it is to bring up these topics in the research. After
all, making research does not justify writing publicly about
things that are too personal and difficult for another person.
Some people might prefer forgetting the negative sides of family
life, and see no point in passing them to further generations,
let alone talking about them to a larger audience.
My (paternal) grandfather
died last April, and at his funerals a veteran spoke about the
critical situations he and my grandfather had faced during the
wartime. My grandfather was a driver in the war, and once this
veteran was driving some truckload to the front line with my
grandfather (during the Continuation war, I think), when Russians
started to fire at them. My grandfather had but stepped on the
gas and driven at full throttle to escape the fire. It was a
narrow escape and according to this veteran it was thanks to
my grandfather's coolness that they survived. When the veteran
told this story at the memorial ceremony of the funeral, it was
a difficult and also a touching moment for both my father and
his siblings. My grandfather had never been willing to speak
about his wartime experiences himself, but it was evident that
it had traumatised him. Sometimes, while slightly drunk, he would
mention something about the war, but otherwise he never talked
about it.
The story of the veteran
brought up something completely new about my grandfather in the
eyes of the whole family; on the other hand it seemed somewhat
to explain his reticence and the reservedness of his nature.
The story is certainly true, although its "hero"-aspect
might have been emphasised on the one hand by reminiscence, and
on the other hand by the thought of honouring my dead grandfather
this way. At the memorial ceremony this story impressed many
people, and on the other hand it also showed that along with
grandfather one generation of our family died, and with him disappeared
also all those memories and stories we would never know. (N16: 4)
Sometimes people do not want
to get back to painful subjects via stories, but for others talking
about negative memories can be therapeutic: they stress that
people do need to speak about difficult things (cf. Anepaio 2001
(7)). My grandmother, for example, says
she has told her children everything that has taken place: "Who
else to trust, if not my own children?" (H8: 47). Talking
about difficult events and subjects sometimes makes things more
understandable or acceptable for one as well (cf. Jaago 1999).
The influence of research
on people participating in it
Also during the writing process
the researcher has to consider which is her closest interest
group - if she is above all a researcher or a family member,
and where the loyalties are to be. Especially when studying her
own group, the researcher has to keep in mind that she cannot
simply pack up and go home when the research is over (Scheiberg
1990: 212). Ethical questions cannot be ignored in any research,
but their importance is especially emphasised when people taking
part in the study have a long common history.
Usually anonymity should be
guaranteed for people taking part in the research, but it is
complicated in case the study strives towards self-reflectivity.
When the researcher reveals something about herself and her feelings,
analyses the stories of her family, she also discloses other
family members and their personal field of life. I have analysed
my own and my family members' experiences using real names with
the consent of family members. However, also people who did not
directly take part in the research (via interviews or questionnaire)
are nonetheless part of it via stories and answers others have
told and given. When trying to get the best out of one's own
experiences, the anonymity of other family members cannot be
maintained either.
While discussing and quoting
written material and the interviews made with other families
I have changed all the names in citations. During the interviews
and when people were handing written answers over to me, it became
clear that people were worried that their personal statements
would reveal their identity to the future reader of the study.
However, nobody's answers have been published as a whole, whereupon
recognition of people is unlikely. Some people have given me
encouraging feedback afterwards. One informant wrote, for example,
that she was pleased to find familiar stories and soul-searching
in my text, becoming aware that her personal experiences and
sensitive thoughts have something to give. At the same time she
writes that her doubts concerning the revelation of her identity
were dispelled; recognition is possible only among family members.
During the research process
I have sometimes pondered my future relationship towards my family,
family storytelling and reminiscing. Have I lost the natural
relationship towards it for good? Will I always think about the
stories as a possible research material and pay attention to
them accordingly? Will my close ones always conceive me as an
'observing family member'? At least at the time when I was doing
research for my master's thesis, many of my friends immediately
made some kind of comment on the subject, if someone told a family
story or referred to family traditions in the course of conversation.
Research process can be considered
as a series of different encounters, which in one way or another
influence all the people taking part in the research. I have
started from the premise that the self and other do not need
to be separated, but can also be exchangeable categories, which
exist in relation to each other. In the participation process
the boundary between the researcher and the informant can be
crossed over. The researcher herself can become the informant
she wishes to study (Camitta 1990: 24-25). In a way the research
process itself can be thought of as a journey or an excursion
to oneself (see also Ruotsala 2001: 119). Owing to this project
I have learned new things about myself, my family and my close
ones. The research process can also give other family members
new perspectives in life and a chance to develop. Still, one
should note that research can also be disturbing, people can
find it unpleasant or annoying, and the researcher should not
be too eager to push others to acquire self-knowledge they did
not seek or want. (8)
References
Anepaio, Terje 2001. Trauma
ja mälu. Mineviku ületamisest represseeritute kogemuses.
(Summary: Trauma and memory: repressed Estonians coping with
past.) - Terje Anepaio & Ene Kõresaar (toim.). Kultuur
ja mälu. Konverentsi materjale. Studia Ethnologia Tartuensia
4. Tartu: Tartu Ülikooli Kirjastus, lk. 198-213.
Camitta, Miriam 1990. Gender
and Method in Folklore Fieldwork. - Southern Folklore, Vol. 47,
No. 1, pp. 21-31.
Clifford, James & Marcus,
George (eds.) 1986. Writing Culture. The Poetics and Politics
of Ethnography. Berkeley, California: University of California
Press.
Holbek, Bengt 1990. The Family
Anecdote: Event and Narrative. - Lutz Röhrich & Sabine
Wienker-Piepho (eds.). Storytelling in Contemporary Societies.
Tybingen: Gunter Narr Verlag, pp. 103-112.
Jaago, Tiiu 1999. Conflict,
Experience and Nostalgia in Family Narratives: On the Example
of Estonia and Finland. - Folklore. No. 12, pp. 71-87. http://haldjas.folklore.ee/folklore/vol12/famnarr/htm
Kippar, Pille 1997. Stories
told in our family. - Ingrid Rüütel & Kristin Kuutma
(eds.). The Family as the Tradition Carrier. Conference Proceedings,
Vol. 2. NIF-publications, No. 31. Tallinn, pp. 74-76.
Miller, Kim 1997. All in the
Family: Family Folklore, Objectivity and Self-Censorship. - Western
Folklore Vol. 56, No. 3 & 4, pp. 331-346.
Olsen, Kjell 2001. Neighbours,
students and informants: 'Fieldwork as an attitude or a lifestyle'.
- Pille Runnel (ed.). Rethinking Ethnology and Folkloristics.
Vanavaravedaja 6. Tartu: Tartu Nefa Rühm, pp. 159-181.
Pink, Sarah 2000. 'Informants'
who come 'home'. - Constructing the Field. Ethnographic Fieldwork
in the Contemporary World. London & New York: Routledge,
pp. 96-119.
Ruotsala, Helena 2001. Fieldwork
at home: Possibilities and limitations of native research. -
Pille Runnel (ed.). Rethinking Ethnology and Folkloristics. Vanavaravedaja
6. Tartu: Tartu Nefa Rühm, pp. 111-131.
Saarinen, Tuija 1993. Heikan
Jussin huumori Herralan kylän perinteen valossa. An unpublished
master's graduate thesis. University of Helsinki.
Scheiberg, Susan L. 1990. A
Folklorist in the Family: On the Process of Fieldwork Among Intimates.
- Western Folklore 49 (April 1990), pp. 208-214.
Sherman, Sharon R. 1986. "That's
How the Seder Looks": A Fieldwork Account of Videotaping
Family Folklore. - Journal of Folklore Research, Vol. 23, No.
1, pp. 53-70.
Stoeltje, Beverly J. &
Fox, Christine L. & Olbrys, Stephen 1999. The Self in "Fieldwork":
A Methodological Concern. - Journal of American Folklore, Vol.
112, No. 444, pp. 158-182.
Suojanen, Päivikki 1995.
Tutkijan kuva kohteesta. Metodologisia näkökohtia.
- Sananjalka, nr. 37. Suomen kielen seuran vuosikirja. Turku:
Suomen kielen seura, s. 147-165.
Vasenkari, Maria 1996. Mitä
se sanoo? Mistä se kertoo? Dialoginen näkökulma
kenttätutkimusaineiston tuottamiseen. - Tuija Hovi &
Lotte Tarkka (toim.). Etiäinen 3. Uskontotiede - Folkloristiikka.
Kirjoituksia opinnäytteistä. Turku: Turun yliopisto,
s. 84-109.
Vilkko, Anni 1995. Lukijaelämää.
- Elina Haavio-Mannila & Tommi Hoikkala & Eeva Peltonen
& Anni Vilkko (toim.). Kerro vain totuus. Elämäkertatutkimuksen
omaelämäkerrallisuus. Helsinki: Gaudeamus, s. 157-172.
Vuorinen, Pihla 2000. "Entäs
muistat sie
" Perhekerronta muisteluna ja merkitysten
arviointina. An unpublished master's graduate thesis University
of Joensuun.
Wilson, William A. 1991. Personal
Narratives: The Family Novel. - Western Folklore, Vol. 50, No.
2, pp. 127-149.
Yocom, Margaret R. 1982. Family
Folklore and Oral History Interviews: Strategies for Introducing
a Project to One's Own Relatives. - Western Folklore, Vol. 41,
No. 4, pp. 251-274.
Yocom, Margaret R. 1997: Family
Folklore. - Thomas A. Green (ed.). Folklore: An Encyclopedia
of Beliefs, Customs, Tales, Music and Art. Vol. I, A-H. Oxford:
Clio, pp. 278-285.
References from text:
(1)
With the word 'family' I refer both to a smaller family unit
and to a larger circle of relatives. Back
(2)
I got 43 answers to my questionnaire in writing. Respondents
were 25 years old on average, and thus my material reflects mostly
the memories, stories and ideas of a younger generation. In addition
to these written answers, my material consists of ten interviews,
seven of which were group interviews, where two or more family
members were present. This material was collected during the
years 1997-1999, and is retained in the archives of Finnish Literary
Society in Joensuu. Back
(3)
In Estonia Pille Kippar has studied the stories of her own family
(e.g. Kippar 1997). Conducting fieldwork as a part of their everyday
life or among their friends or neighbours has also been discussed
by e.g. Kjell Olsen (2001), Sarah Pink (2000) and Helena Ruotsala
(2001). Back
(4)
Following the example of Margaret Yocom (1982), I have decided
to use pronoun 'she' throughout this paper, "as a universal
pronoun much more comfortable to myself as the author".
Back
(5)
References to my material are in a form N15, referring to a certain
woman (N1-N34) or a man (M1-M9) who has answered my questionnaire,
or to an interview (H1-H10). Back
(6)
Sometimes I know even too well what to ask, and my family members
know that I know (cf. Ruotsala 2001: 124). As a consequence they
may feel that they have no choice between whether to tell their
'secrets' to me or not. Back
(7)
Anepaio (2001) has studied the relationship to the past of those
Estonians, who lived through the repressions in the 1940s. Some
of the respondents avoided transmission of tragic memories to
the next generation, others again say that they have told their
children 'everything'. According to Anepaio, the differences
in willingness to talk about the past partly depend on how the
person in question has worked through the experienced herself.
Back
(8)
Cf. Ethical Guidelines for Good Research Practice. Association
of Social Anthropologists of the Commonwealth. (http://les1.man.ac.uk/asa/ethics.htm). Back
Contents